All
foreign language learners struggle with their accents when learning a new
language. As many are aware it seems nearly impossible for a second language
learner to speak the language with a native-level accent. However, it does
appear possible, even as a “late language learner” to develop a native-level
accent in a foreign language. Some research indicates that children under the
age of twelve have a much easier time overcoming their native accents in
foreign languages than “late language learners,” or those who have started
learning the new language after the age of twelve (McDonald, 2006). It is
important as language teachers to realize this and to perhaps develop methods
for instilling native-level accents in all students.
Some studies have found that the attainment of a
native-level accent for all ages is ultimately impossible. For example, Flege et al., in 2006, performed a study on
native Korean children and adults who had been living in the United States
between three and five years. They found that both children and adults still
had foreign accents when compared to native English speakers of the same ages
(Flege et al., 2006). In 1995, Flege et al. performed a similar study
involving 240 native Italians who had moved to Canada, and been living there
for a period of thirty-two years. He found with this group, both the children,
who had arrived in Canada well within the Critical Period (ages 3-11), and
those who had arrived as late language learners, outside the critical period,
had distinctive foreign accents when compared to native English speakers (Flege
et al., 1995). This research indicates
that when learning a new language the student is doomed to have a foreign
accent.
When
exploring further research the results become more varied, and some studies
even show native-level accents to be quite possible in foreign language
learners. Singleton and Lengyel performed a study in 1995 on second language
acquisition of English in two groups of native Dutch, all of whom were late
learners, one group of “exceptionally successful learners of English,” and one
group of average learners of English (Singleton, 1995). They were recorded and
tested on four different speech patterns. First, talking for three minutes
about their most recent holiday abroad, second, read aloud a short English text
(84 words), third, read aloud ten short English sentences (5-10 words), and
fourth, read aloud twenty-five English words. These two groups were compared to
a group of five native English speakers, who were asked to perform the same
four patterns, and all were rated on a scale from one to five, one, a very
strong foreign accent, and five, a definitively native accent. Perhaps the most
exciting piece of evidence that was found in this research was that on average
the “exceptionally successful learners of English” out performed the native
English speakers with a mean score of 4.31 compared to 3.94 (Singleton, 1995). This
shows ability in foreign language learners to develop native-level accents in
English. Neufeld conducted an
experiment with twenty Canadian university students over the course of about
twenty hours. The group was divided in half, and ten students learned some
basic Chinese sound patterns, and the other ten learned some basic Japanese
sound patterns. This took place over a total of eighteen hours. Then each
student was given ten short phrases in the target language and asked to repeat
it five times. The last repetition was recorded and played to native speakers
of the target language. Nine subjects were judged to be native speakers of
Japanese, and eight subjects were judged to be native speakers of Chinese
(Singleton, 1995). This indicates that native-level accents are attainable to
foreign language learners if they focus on learning the basic sound patters of
a language before focusing on vocabulary.
The
Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH) has been used to describe difficulties in acquiring
a native-level accent in a foreign language. In 1967 Eric Lenneberg proposed
the CPH, which states that there is a period of cognitive and physical
development, ages five to puberty, in which language learning progresses more
rapidly, and after which it is more difficult to develop (Lenneberg, 1967). This
hypothesis was derived from: evidence of feral children and victims of child
abuse who were raised without exposure to human language and who were unable to
fully acquire the ability to produce it, deaf children who were unable to
develop spoken language after puberty, and evidence that children with aphasia
have a better chance at recovery than aphasiac adults (Lenneberg, 1967).
However, given the research and studies above, this theory does not hold true
in regards to foreign language acquisition and acquiring native-level accents.
Teachers
can use various activities to address the basic sound patterns of a language in
tandem with teaching students vocabulary and sentence structure. Listening to
music in the target language is a way of exposing the basic sound patterns of
the target language, and the use of other media such as television shows,
movies, or skits may be used as well to illustrate those sound patterns in more
a natural setting. Another activity to do for short periods of time (about five
minutes) is to use similar sounds to address correct pronunciation. For
example, if a student has trouble with the word ‘mouse,’ but can pronounce
‘house’ perfectly, alternating between the two words quickly improves the
pronunciation of the difficult word. Pronunciation quizzes or speaking
exercises can be used frequently to evaluate the students’ progress towards
native-level accents as well. These tools may help students attain accents closer
to native-level, however, given the evidence of the studies above, there is no
evidence to support that any activities will be more or less effective at
teaching accents.
Works Cited
- Flege, James E. et al. Effects of age of second-language learning on the production of English consonants. Speech Communication, Vol. 16. 1995.
- Flege, James E. et al. Degree of foreign accent in English sentences produced by Korean children and adults. Journal of Phonetics, Vol. 34. 2006.
- Lenneberg, Eric. Biological Foundations of Language. Wiley, New York. 1967.
- McDonald, Janet L. Beyond the Critical Period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammatically judgement performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, Vol. 55, Iss. 3. October 2006.
- Singleton, David and Zesolt Lengyen. The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition. Bristol, PA: Multilingual Matters, 1995. Book.
Well said Ben. We have had several preschool kids who did not speak any English. Music, making sure they could see your lips move, saying things over in several ways, being expressive in hands and face, and showing them pictures really helped. But the best by far was music. BMcG.
ReplyDelete